Aggression,
hypermasculine emotions and relations: the
silence/violence pattern
THOMAS SCHEFF
University of
California, Santa Barbara
Boys learn early that
showing vulnerable feelings (grief, fear and shame) are seen
as signs of weakness. First
at home, then at school they find that acting out anger,
even if faked, is seen as
strength. Expressing anger verbally, rather than storming,
may be seen as weakness. At
first merely to protect themselves, boys begin
suppressing feelings that
may be interpreted as signs of weakness.
In
Western cultures most boys learn, as first option, to hide their vulnerable
feelings in emotionless
talk, withdrawal, or silence. I will call these three
responses (emotional)
SILENCE. In situations where these options seem unavail-
able, males may cover their
vulnerable feelings behind a display of hostility. That
is, young boys learn in
their families, and later, from their peers, to suppress emo-
tions they actually feel by
acting out one emotion, anger, whether they feel it or not.
I
call this pattern Ôsilence/violenceÕ. Vulnerable feelings are first hidden from
others, and after many
repetitions, even from self. In this latter stage, behaviour
becomes compulsive. When
men face what they construe to be threatening situa-
tions, they may be
compelled to SILENCE or to rage and aggression.
Even
without threat, men seem to be more likely to SILENCE or violence than
women. With their partners,
most men are less likely to talk freely than women
about feelings of resentment,
humiliation, embarrassment, rejection, joy, genuine
pride, loss and anxiety.
This may be the reason they are more likely to show anger:
they seem to be backed up
on a wide variety of intense feelings, but sense that only
anger is allowed them. The
phrase Ôbacked upÕ was first used by Tomkins (see selec-
tions from his work in the
volume edited by Demos 1995, pp. 92–4, 57, 275–6).
Why
did Tomkins use such an award phrase, rather than the more obvious
choice: Ôrepressed?Õ To
understand his choice requires a brief digression into the
history of psychology
during the period that he was writing, in the sixties and
seventies. There was little
hard evidence for or against the concepts of repression
and the unconscious at this
time, and not much more today. By and large, most
psychotherapists assumed it
to be true and academic psychologists assumed that it
was not true. Indeed,
academic psychologists ridiculed these ideas, especially the
idea that emotions exerted
ÔhydraulicÕ pressure on everyday life.
In
this context, Tomkins did not use terms like repression and unconscious,
perhaps in an attempt to
avoid open conflict with the vast majority of his
colleagues. But his system
assumes the repression of painful emotions to the point
that they become
unconscious in everyday life. Although himself an academic
psychologist, he found it
necessary to invent terms that would allow his theory of
emotions to involve
repression and the unconscious emotions that result.
My
own view of emotions is based largely on my experiences as a teacher,
marriage counsellor
(1971–76), and my own personal life. For the last thirty-five
years of teaching, my
classes came close to being forms of group psychotherapy,
even the large classes.
Although I never called attention to the similarity, students
often did. Usually the
comments they made in this regard were approving; most of
them thought it added to
the value of the class. The format of my classes, whatever
their official names,
basically involved having the students examine their own
experiences, to help them
understand their emotional/relational worlds.
During
the period of student activism against the Vietnam War, these classes
became intensely emotional.
In a large course titled Interpersonal Relations, taught
many times over a period of
three years, students underwent mass weeping and
laughing, both in the large
meetings, small discussion groups, and in office visits
by groups of students. In
1979 I received the Distinguished Teaching Award from
the UCSB Academic Senate
largely on the basis of these classes. Most of my views
on emotional/relational
issues were formed by my close contacts with thousands
of students.
My
personal life has also been dense with emotional/relational issues. Between
the ages of 14 and 40 I
certainly fitted the pattern of male repression of vulnerable
emotions. I had learned to
be a strong and silent male like my father, and that
expressions of fear, grief
and shame at school made me prey to bullies. Although
I have no memory of my dad
equating fear with cowardice, it was implied in his
comments and actions. Over
the course of childhood, I seem to have gradually
numbed out feelings of
fear. In my late 30s, during the Vietnam protest, I took many
risks that seem shockingly
unacceptable to me now. Some of my colleagues compli-
mented me on my courage,
but looking back it seems to me I was merely reckless.
Numbing
out fear, particularly, makes men dangerous to themselves and others.
Fear is an innate signal of
danger that helps us survive. When we see a car heading
toward us on a collision
course, we have an immediate, automatic fear response:
WAKE UPSLEEPY-HEAD, YOUR
LIFE IS IN DANGER! Much faster than
thought, this reaction
increases our chance of survival, and repressing it is dan-
gerous to self and others.
If the sense of fear has been repressed, it is necessary to
find ways of uncovering it.
Although
the idea is only hinted at in Tomkins, it now seems likely that repres-
sion of emotions leads to a
vicious circle. One represses emotions in order to avoid
painful feelings. At first
the painful feelings have their origins in the reactions of
others, especially our
parents and schoolmates. Certainly as a child I sensed that my
father saw expressions of
grief or fear as indicating weakness. He often used a
Yiddish expression in these
circumstances: ÔZai ayne menschÕ. At the time I took
it to mean ÔBe a
manÕ(instead of acting like a baby). What was painful to me was
less the words (which
actually mean ÔBe a real personÕ) than his signs of
impatience and even disgust
at my behaviour.
In
order to avoid pain inflicted by others, we learn to repress the expressions of
feeling that lead to
negative reactions from others. After thousands of curtail-
ments, repression becomes
habitual and out of consciousness. But as we become
more backed up with avoided
emotions, we have the sense that experienced them
would be unbearably
painful. In this way, avoidance leads to avoidance in an ever
increasing, self-perpetuating
loop.
For
a lengthy period as a teenager and young man, it never occurred to me to try
to identify and talk about
the various feelings I might have had. I was angry much
of the time, and sometimes
enraged. As my son later told me, my anger was unpre-
dictable. It was a problem
in all of my relationships.
However,
at the age of 40, both by accident and through various forms of
therapy, I began to learn
how to cry and feel fear, rather than numb it out. My first
experience of intense crying
at this age led to a solid year of crying every day,
without exception. It was
as if I had a backlog of tears to deal with.
My
experiences of fear were different, however. They were only two of them,
but they were profound,
about six months apart. The first occurred as a result of
therapy, after intense
episodes of crying and laughing. The second was triggered by
a death threat on the phone
from an irate citizen. During this time I was both chair
of an academic department
and an anti-war activist. This combination increased my
visibility, and it
irritated a lot of people, both in and outside of the university.
Both
fear episodes were quite similar in content and in duration. They each
lasted about twenty
minutes, and involved what would have looked like epileptic
seizures from the outside.
As I lay on the floor, my body went through convulsive
shaking with an
earthquake-like intensity, and sweating that soaked my clothes as
if I had been swimming in
them. Unlike my crying episodes, there was no mental
content associated with the
two fits of fear. Also, unlike the crying, which occurred
so easily as to become
commonplace, I felt utterly transformed after each fear
episode.
These
fear experiences also had an immediately visible effect. After the second
one, I actually began to
experience fear when I was in danger. Since I was still
deeply involved in the
Vietnam protest, I began to be less reckless. Isla Vista, the
student community where
most of my activity took place, was an extremely
dangerous place at this
time. At times the student protesters and the police were in
open warfare. My change
with respect to fear probably helped protect me and
other protesters from
injury.
Surprisingly,
neither the crying nor the fear episodes were painful. Indeed, they
were more pleasurable than
painful. In the fear response, particularly, I felt some-
what like a child on a
delicious roller-coaster ride. Apparently all of these changes
occurred at what I have
called optimal distance (in my theory of catharsis: Scheff,
1979). That is, I was both
in a state of grief or fear, but also outside it, looking on
like a member of an
audience in a theatre.
Making
the acquaintance of my own shame came later, with more difficulty. At
any rate, episodes of anger
and rage became less frequent, briefer, and less intense
as I learned to identify
and feel vulnerable emotions. Another decisive step in this
direction occurred as a
result of marriage to my present wife, Suzanne Retzinger.
After we began living
together, she would usually come home from her job as a
mediator in a child custody
court, laden with talk. She would go on for what often
seemed to me an
interminable time, reviewing events of her day at work. Sometimes
she would recount the same
event several times. Listening to this daily drama, I
was rapidly becoming
exasperated.
However,
after several months of suffering in silence, I noticed that she usu-
ally seemed to feel much
better after her marathon of talk. Anew thought occurred
to me: if it works for her,
maybe it will work for me! So we took turns review-
ing the events of our day.
At first I could hardly fill five minutes, much less the
45 that Suzanne usually
took. But with some patient probing and questions
on her part, I learned how
to go over the events of my day, finding and trying to
finish unfinished
emotion-laden events. As I learned to do that, I began to feel
better. On the basis of my
own experiences and as a teacher, I have come to
believe that everyone needs
to experience the full range of their emotions if they
are to thrive.
Gender differences in
emotion management
In my experience, most
women express vulnerable emotions more fully than
most men. Certainly they
express fear and grief more. The difference between
men and women with respect
to shame is probably smaller, but with women still
more expressive of this
emotion, if only obliquely. That is, women seem more
likely to review the events
of their day, either to themselves or with another
person, than men. In doing
so, they are likely to encounter one or more of the
vulnerable emotions.
On
the other hand, more women are inhibited about expressing anger, whether
verbally or acting it out.
Each year of teaching hundred students about emotions, I
would come across at least
one female student who claimed never to have felt
anger. This student usually
wore a continuous smile that was difficult to remove,
even on request. When such
a student did hit upon the experience of anger during
the course exercises, she
appeared both alarmed and delighted.
My
impression is that the gender difference in these four emotions is slowly
decreasing, as women are
being prepared at home and school for careers. This
change is clearest with
respect to anger; more women are expressing anger either
verbally or by acting out.
The change toward the masculine pattern of vulnerable
emotions is less clear, and
may be quite slow. It seems that even career women
still cry much more freely
than men and are quicker to acknowledge fear.
Studies
of unresolved grief and of alexthymia (Krystal, 1988) indirectly sup-
port the different
management of emotions by men and women. Alexithymia is a
recent addition to
diagnostic categories, meaning absence of feeling and emotion.
Unresolved grief is an
older diagnosis. Unlike most psychiatric diagnoses, there is
almost unanimous agreement
that this syndrome is one whose, Ôcause is known,
whose features are
distinctive, and whose course is predictableÕ(Parkes, 1998).
At
any rate, although these studies do not comment on gender differences, in the
case studies reported, men
outnumber women by a ratio of about four to one. A
patient who shows up in a
psychiatristÕs office with symptoms of alexithymia or
unresolved grief is much
more likely to be a man than a woman.
Doka
and Martin (1998) have argued that menÕs grieving is not recognised as
such, because it is largely
cognitive and behavioural, rather than affective. In this
and other publications,
Doka has sought to back up his idea with empirical data.
But it seems to me that his
data, based on paper and pencil inventories, hardly
touches the realities of
grieving. However, his idea that grieving has cognitive and
behavioural, as well as
emotional components is probably valid. And not just for
grief, but also for fear
and shame also: talking about feeling has a role in reframing
trauma that is partially
independent of feeling.
The
difference between men and womenÕs attitudes toward violence can be
seen in the various polls
that are relevant to the support of the Iraq war. No matter
which poll or the framing
of the question, women always express less support for
the war. Women are much
less keen on violence than men in its collective form. At
the level of families,
women are also much less likely to commit violence than
men, especially physical
violence.
A
recent literature review of responses to stress (Taylor et al., 2000) finds
that
women, much more than men,
are likely to Ôtend-and-befriendÕ rather than fight-or-
flight. The
attachment/networking response seems to be more alive in women than
in men. The tend/befriend
pattern can be viewed as the default variant for females,
an important modification
of CannonÕs idea of fight or flight.
This
paper proposes that the silence/violence pattern is the corresponding
variant for males. The
violence part obviously corresponds to fight. But the silence
part is equivalent to
flight, if withdrawal includes not just physical flight, but also
withdrawal in its
psychological sense. The Taylor et al. Ôtend/befriendÕ pattern for
women, when combined with
the silence/violence pattern for men suggests that the
fight/flight response is
crucially modified by culturally driven gender differences.
The
way in which the US military continues its policy of discrimination against
gays, in defiance of court
rulings, suggests the crucial role that hypermasculinity
plays in collective
violence. But the evidence is indirect. The role of hypermas-
culine emotions in actual
events is difficult to evaluate directly because of
inadequate reporting of the
emotional/relational world.
Conventional
reporting involves the behavioural/cognitive world, at best. But
the nature of the emotions
involved, and relationships, can be inferred from these
materials if they are
interpreted within the larger context. This method is first
applied to the case of
William Calley, the Army officer convicted of ordering and
helping carry out the
massacre at My Lai, and then to the much fuller accounts of
HitlerÕs life. But there is
one dramatic difference between the two that makes
CalleyÕs behaviour seem
almost as disturbing as HitlerÕs: even though he
organised the murder of
millions, Hitler is not known to have to have ever killed
even one of those that he
led others to kill. Calley not only ordered murder, but
killed many of his victims
himself.
William Calley and the
My Lai massacre
This account is based on
several sources. The first is the online record of a PBS
broadcast: The American
Experience: Vietnam (PBS, undated).
The second is
based on a recent review of
CalleyÕs conviction for murder, within the larger
perspective of the US
military involvement in the Vietnam war (Belknap, 2002).
Other biographies are also
cited: Hersh, 1970; Calley, 1971; Everett, 1971;
Greenshaw, 1971; Hammer,
1971.
Charley
Company reached Mai Lai village on 16 March, 1968, led by
Lt. William Calley. Like
some of the men serving under him, CalleyÕs back-
ground was unheroic. (The
following account is an abbreviated version of the
PBS text.)
[His] utter lack of respect
for the indigenous population was apparent to all
in the company. According
to one soldier, Ôif they wanted to do something
wrong, it was all right
with CalleyÕ. Seymour Hersh wrote that by March of
1968 Ômany in the company
had given in to an easy pattern of violenceÕ.
Soldiers systematically
beat unarmed civilians. Some civilians were murdered.
Whole villages were burned.
Wells were poisoned. Rapes were common.
On
March 14, a small squad from ÔCÕ Company ran into a booby trap,
killing a popular sergeant,
blinding one GI and wounding several others.
The following evening, when
a funeral service was held for the killed ser-
geant, soldiers had revenge
on their mind. After the service, Captain Medina
rose to give the soldiers a
pep talk and discuss the next morningÕs mission.
Medina told them that the
VC were in the vicinity of a hamlet known as My
Lai 4, which would be the
target of a large-scale assault by the company.
The
soldiersÕ mission would be to engage the enemy and to destroy the
village of My Lai. By 7
a.m., Medina said, the women and children would be
out of the hamlet and all
they could expect to encounter would be the enemy.
The soldiers were to
explode brick homes, set fire to thatch homes, shoot
livestock, poison wells,
and destroy the enemy. The seventy-five or so
American soldiers would be
supported in their assault by gunship pilots.
Medina
later said that his objective that night was to Ôfire them up and
get them ready to go in
there; I did not give any instructions as to what to do
with women and children in
the villageÕ. Although some soldiers agreed
with that recollection of
MedinaÕs, others clearly thought that he had ordered
them to kill every person
in My Lai 4. Perhaps his orders were intentionally
vague. What seems likely is
that Medina intentionally gave the impression
that everyone in My Lai
would be their enemy.
At
7:22 a.m. on March 16, nine helicopters lifted off for the flight to My
Lai 4. By the time the
helicopters carrying members of Charlie Company
landed in a rice paddy
about 140 yards south of My Lai, the area had been
peppered with small arms
fire from assault helicopters. Whatever VC might
have been in the vicinity
of My Lai had most likely left by the time the first
soldiers climbed out of
their helicopters. The assault plan called for Lt.
CalleyÕs first platoon and
Lt. Stephen BrooksÕs second platoon to sweep into
the village, while a third
platoon, Medina, and the headquarters unit would
be held in reserve and
follow the first two platoons in after the area was
more or less secured.
My
Lai village had about 700 residents. They lived in either redbrick
homes or thatch-covered
huts. Adeep drainage ditch marked the eastern
boundary of the village.
Directly south of the residential area was an open
plaza area used for holding
village meetings. To the north and west of the
village was dense foliage.
By
8 a.m., CalleyÕs platoon had crossed the plaza on the townÕs southern
edge and entered the
village. They encountered families cooking rice in
front of their homes. The
men began their usual search-and-destroy task of
pulling people from homes,
interrogating them, and searching for VC. Soon
the killing began. The
first victim was a man stabbed in the back with a
bayonet. Then a middle-aged
man was picked up, thrown down a well, and
a grenade lobbed in after
him. Agroup of fifteen to twenty mostly older
women were gathered around
a temple, kneeling and praying. They were all
executed with shots to the
back of their heads.
Eighty
or so villagers were taken from their homes and herded to the
plaza area. As many cried
ÔNo VC! No VC!Õ, Calley told soldier Paul
Meadlo ÔYou know what I
want you to do with themÕ. When Calley returned
ten minutes later and found
the Vietnamese still gathered in the plaza he
reportedly said to Meadlo,
ÔHavenÕt you got rid of them yet? I want them
dead. Waste themÕ. Meadlo
and Calley began firing into the group from a
distance of ten to fifteen
feet. The few that survived did so because they
were covered by the bodies
of those less fortunate.
What
Captain Medina knew of these war crimes is not certain. It was a
chaotic operation. Gary
Garfolo said, ÔI could hear shooting all the time.
Medina was running back and
forth everywhere. This wasnÕt no organised
dealÕ. Medina would later
testify that he didnÕt enter the village until 10 a.m.,
after most of the shooting
had stopped, and did not personally witness a
single civilian being killed.
Others put Medina in the village closer to 9 a.m.,
and close to the scene of
many of the murders as they were happening.
As
the third platoon moved into My Lai, it was followed by army
photographer Ronald
Haeberle, there to document what was supposed to be
a significant encounter
with a crack enemy battalion. Haeberle took many
pictures. He said he saw
about thirty different GIs kill about 100 civilians.
Once Haeberle focused his
camera on a young child about five feet away,
but before he could get his
picture the kid was blown away. He angered
some GIs as he tried to
photograph them as they fondled the breasts of a
fifteen-year-old Vietnamese
girl.
Meanwhile,
the rampage below continued. Calley was at the drainage ditch
on the eastern edge of the
village, where about seventy to eighty old men,
women, and children not
killed on the spot had been brought. Calley ordered
the dozen or so platoon
members there to push the people into the ditch, and
three or four GIs did.
Calley ordered his men to shoot into the ditch. Some
refused, others obeyed. One
who followed CalleyÕs order was Paul Meadlo,
who estimated that he
killed about twenty-five civilians. (Later Meadlo was
seen, head in hands,
crying). Calley joined in the massacre. At one point,
a two-year-old child who
somehow survived the gunfire began running
towards the hamlet. Calley
grabbed the child, threw him back in the ditch,
then shot him.
In prior studies (1990) of
massacres like the one in My Lai, the most prominent
hypothesis concerns what
has been called Ôa forward panicÕ. This idea proposes
that any group in a highly
emotional state, especially a state of fear, is capable of
massacre. The parallel upon
which this idea is based is the behaviour of audiences
in theatre fires. In a
panic to get out of the theatre, members of the audience may
trample on each other.
Apanic state of this kind leads to unintentional, indeed
compulsive behaviour.
Atelling detail from these accounts is that many audience
members seem to have no memory
of the panic. In their desperation to flee the
theatre, they may have
experienced an absence, which is
French for temporarily
losing your mind.
The
idea of panic seems to explain collective behaviour in theatre fires very
well. Apanic suggests flight
behaviour driven entirely by a single emotion, fear,
and that it has no basis in
the previous history of the members of the crowd.
Forward panic adds a new
idea, that instead of flight, panic can also lead to fight.
In the case of massacres,
fight would take the form of slaughter.
There
are several studies of massacres by soldiers that strongly suggest forward
panics (Collins, 1990).
Military units that had no history of earlier violence, under
conditions of great danger,
have committed mayhem, either captive enemy soldiers
or helpless civilians. In
CollinÕs forthcoming study of collective violence, he suggests
that the slaughter at My
Lai may have been caused, at least in part, by forward panic.
While
there are some indications of forward panic in the massacre at My Lai,
there are many indications
that suggest other causes as well. The prior history of the
behaviour of the soldiers
in Company C is rife with episodes of earlier violence
against civilians,
suggesting a habitual pattern of behaviour as one of the causes of
My Lai. There are also many
suggestions that point toward intentionality by Calley
and by his superior
officers, including his immediate superior, Capt. Medina. Both
the orders from above and
CalleyÕs actions themselves can be seen as intentional.
Although MedinaÕs orders
are not completely unambiguous, certainly CalleyÕs
comments and actions
suggest intention, rather than compulsive actions during
a panic.
Another,
more obvious limitation of the forward panic hypothesis is that there
seem to have been other
emotions involved, in addition to fear. It seems obvious
that fear was a part of the
pattern. In the events leading up to My Lai, Company C
had been exposed to grave
and constant danger. They were fighting an enemy that
was virtually invisible,
attacking under thick forest cover, and in silence. The lives
of these soldiers had been
on the line 24/7 for many days. Surely they were living
in fear of their lives.
But
the account above suggests other emotions as well. The US soldiers found
the skilful tactics of
their enemy frustrating, which is one of many vernacular ways
of implicating the emotion
of anger. Anger is also implied in regard to the death of
one of their sergeants and
the wounding of several of their fellows, only two days
before the arrival at My
Lai: Ô[The] soldiers had revenge on their mindÕ. The idea
of revenge involves not
only anger, since revenge implies a shame-anger sequence.
The inability of the men to
even find, much less defeat the enemy appears to have
given rise not only to fear
and anger, but also to the feeling of defeat and its
consequence, humiliation.
Neither
CalleyÕs autobiographical statement (1971) nor his biographies are
sufficiently detailed to
allow a clear analysis of his emotional life. With the excep-
tion of a temporary bond
with his older sister, he appeared to have formed no close
bonds with anyone. Even
though lacking in details, his biographies do uniformly
suggest conditions for one
emotion, the emotion of shame. Judging from his history,
beginning as a high school
student and extending into his life after leaving school,
he had encountered a long
and virtually uninterrupted series of scornful treatments
from others and unremitting
failures.
Calley
failed many courses in high school and college, and failed at many jobs
after leaving school. By
some monstrous error, when he enlisted in the Army, he
was chosen for OfficersÕ
Candidate School. But his record both in OCS and in his
regular service was one of
failure and scorn. The officer who was his immediate
superior in Vietnam, Capt.
Medina, is recorded as never referring to him by his
name, but instead used only
scornful epithets. For example, in front of his platoon,
Medina referred to Calley as
ÔLt. ShitheadÕ.
Given
this record of unremitting scorn and failure, it is instructive to read
CalleyÕs version of his
life (as told to John Sacks, 1973). Calley was utterly silent
about his long history of
failure and scorn. The difference between the biographies
and CalleyÕs version of his
life would seem to support the idea that violent men
suppress their emotional
lives.
CalleyÕs
behaviour during the massacre itself provides a vivid image of the
silence/violence pattern.
While ordering and participating in the murder of women
and children, he was
emotionally silent. Note the details in the final paragraph
above (PBS, undated):
Calley was at the drainage
ditch on the eastern edge of the village, where
about seventy to eighty old
men, women, and children not killed on the spot
had been brought. Calley
ordered the dozen or so platoon members there to
push the people into the
ditch, and three or four GIs did. Calley ordered his
men to shoot into the
ditch. Some refused, others obeyed. One who followed
CalleyÕs order was Paul
Meadlo, who estimated that he killed about twenty-
five civilians. (Later
Meadlo was seen, head in hands, crying.) Calley joined
in the massacre. At one
point, a two-year-old child who somehow survived
the gunfire began running
towards the hamlet. Calley grabbed the child,
threw him back in the
ditch, then shot him.
It should be noted that
some of his troops refused to obey CalleyÕs murderous
commands, and that one who
did obey (Meadlo) was seen crying afterwards.
CalleyÕs behaviour stands
out not only because of its violence, but because it was
so unemotional. There were
undoubtedly many other massacres in Vietnam simi-
lar to the one at My Lai,
some of them unreported. But even the reported ones
received little attention
compared to My Lai. Perhaps CalleyÕs combination of
emotional silence and
flagrant violence made it so inhuman and repugnant that
there was no way of
avoiding it.
Many
studies of battlefield behaviour have shown that to kill effectively, soldiersÕ
greatest struggle is with
their own conscience. Their personal morality dictates it
wrong to kill other human
beings, even enemy soldiers. But Calley came to battle
with the conscience problem
long overcome: he had numbed out not only fear and
grief, but also feelings of
shame, the basic ingredient of conscience.
The silence/violence
pattern in HitlerÕs biographies
The evidence for unresolved
grief is indirect: there is not a single mention of Hitler
crying, not even as a
child. There are a host of indications, however, that he prized
manliness, strength, and
fortitude in the face of adversity. All of these indications
run counter to placing any
value on crying or other expressions of grief.
HitlerÕs
ideal of iron strength was not merely ideological, since he had distin-
guished himself as a good
soldier in World War I (see below). His courage under
fire may also suggest the
numbing out of fear, since it is difficult to distinguish
between courage and the
mere absence of fear.
The
Swiss psychoanalyst Alice Miller (1983) has suggested a family origin of
HitlerÕs psychopathology,
the conjunction of the fatherÕs physical/emotional vio-
lence and his motherÕs
complicity in it. Miller argues that the rage and shame
caused by his fatherÕs
treatment might have been completely repressed because of
his motherÕs complicity.
Although she pampered Hitler and professed to love him,
she did not protect him
from his fatherÕs wrath, or allow Adolf to express his
feelings about it.
HitlerÕs
mother, Klara, as much as Adolf, was tyrannised by her husband, but
offered only obedience and
respect in return. Because of his motherÕs ÔloveÕ for
him, as a young child,
Adolf was required not only to suffer humiliation by his
father in silence, but also
to respect him for it, a basic context for repression.
In
later years Hitler (1927) was to gloss over his treatment by his parents,
which is congruent with
repression. He described his father as stern but respected,
his childhood as that of a
ÔmotherÕs darling living in a soft downy bedÕ(Bromberg
and Small, 1983: 40).
However, AloisÕs son, Alois Jr, left home at 14 because of his
fatherÕs harshness. His
son, William Patrick, reported that Alois, Sr. beat Alois, Jr.
with a whip. Alois Jr.Õs
first wife, Brigid, reported that Alois Sr. frequently beat
the children, and on
occasion his wife Klara (Bromberg and Small, 1983, 32–3).
It
would appear that HitlerÕs early childhood constituted an external feeling trap
from which there was no
escape. This external trap is the analogue to the internal
trap proposed by Lewis
(1971): when shame is evoked but goes unacknowledged,
it generates intense
symptoms of mental illness and/or violence towards self or
others. Under the
conditions of complete repression that seem to have obtained,
HitlerÕs personality was
grossly distorted. His biographies suggest that he was
constantly in a state of
anger bound by shame.
One
indication of HitlerÕs continual shame/rage was his temper tantrums.
Although in later life some
of them may have been staged, there is no question
that in most of his
tantrums he was actually out of control. His older stepbrother
reported (Gilbert, 1950,
18) that even before he was seven, ÔHitler was imperious
and quick to anger ... If
he didnÕt get his way he got very angry. He would fly into
a rage over any
trivialityÕ. In his teens, HitlerÕs rages were frequent and intense,
evoking such expressions as
Ôred with rageÕ, Ôexceedingly violent and high-
strungÕ, and Ôlike a
volcano eruptingÕ(Kubizek, 1955).
HitlerÕs
compulsive anger is suggested by the slightness of provocation that
triggered rage. KubizekÕs
memoir provides two examples: one occasion on
learning that he had failed
to win a lottery, another when he saw ÔStephanieÕ with
other men. Stephanie was a
girl who Hitler longed to meet, but never did. He
was infatuated with her,
but never introduced himself (Bromberg and Small, 1983:
55–6).
The
most obvious manifestations of HitlerÕs shame occurred after he became
Chancellor. Although easily
the most powerful and admired man in Germany, he
was constantly apprehensive
(Bromberg and Small, 1983: 183): ÔHis anxieties lest
he appear ridiculous, weak,
vulnerable, incompetent, or in any way inferior are
indications of his endless
battle with shame.Õ Further manifestations of chronic
shame states occurred in
his relationships with women. In attempting to interest a
woman in himself,
even the presence of other
persons would not prevent him from repulsive
grovelling. [He would] tell
a lady that he was unworthy to sit near her or kiss
her hand but hoped she
would look on him with favour ... one woman
reported that after all
kinds of self-accusations he said that he was unworthy
of being in the same room
with her. (Bromberg and Small, 1983: 83)
These latter descriptions
of HitlerÕs shame states suggest overt, undifferentiated
shame, emotionally painful
states involving feelings of inadequacy and inferiority.
How then is one to
understand the other side of HitlerÕs personality, his arrogance,
boldness, and extreme
self-confidence? How could a man so shame-prone also be
so shameless?
LewisÕs
(1971) conception of the bimodal nature of unacknowledged shame
provides an answer. In
addition to the overt shame states discussed above, Hitler
also had a long history of
bypassed shame. Many aspects of his behaviour suggest
bypassed shame, but I will
review only three: his temper tantrums, his Ôpiercing
stareÕ(Bromberg and Small,
1983: 309) and his obsessiveness.
As
already indicated, shame theory suggests that protracted and destructive
anger is generated by
unacknowledged shame. Normal anger, when not intermixed
with shame, is usually
brief, moderate, and can even be constructive, serving to
call notice to adjustments
needed in a relationship (Retzinger, 1991). Long chains
of shame and anger
alternating are experienced as blind rage, hatred or resentment
if the shame component is
completely repressed. In this case, the expression of
anger serves as a disguise
for the hidden shame, projecting onto the outside world
the feelings that go
unacknowledged within. According to Lewis (1971), persons
in whom shame is deeply
repressed Ôwould rather turn the world upside down than
turn themselves inside
outÕ. This idea exactly captures the psychology of HitlerÕs
lifelong history of intense
rage states, and his projection of his inner conflict on to
scapegoats in the external
world.
The
second indicator of bypassed shame is HitlerÕs demeanour, especially the
nature of his gaze. As early
as l6, it was described as ÔblankÕ or ÔcruelÕ(Bromberg
and Small, 1983: 51). On
the other hand, there are descriptions at a later time (21)
in which he was said to
have Ôan evasive mannerÕ, of being ÔshyÕ and Ônever looking
a person in the eyeÕ, except
when he was talking politics (ibid.: 70). These descrip-
tions suggest that Hitler
may have been in a virtually permanent state of shame,
manifested as either
bypassed shame (the stare) or overt shame (avoiding eye
contact). As his power
increased, the bypassed mode was more and more in evi-
dence, in the form of
arrogance, extreme self-confidence, isolation, and obsession.
The
prison psychiatrist Gilligan (1996) studied the emotions of male prisoners
convicted of violence. He
found evidence that each of them harboured a kind of
shame similar to HitlerÕs.
GilliganÕs term for it is not unacknowledged or bypassed,
but ÔsecretÕ. He proposed
that secret shame was a fundamental basis for the vio-
lence of these men.
Isolation from others
The biographies and
psychological studies emphasise HitlerÕs isolation as a child
and adult (Bromberg and
Small, 1983; Bullock, 1964; Davidson, 1977; Miller,
1983; Stierlin, 1976). As
an infant and youth, he was pampered by his mother. But
even as young as three, the
relationship with his father was charged with violence,
ridicule, and contempt. By
the age of six, he apparently was walled off from every-
one, including his mother
(Bromberg and Small, 1983; Miller, 1983; Stierlin, 1976).
The
three most likely candidates for a close relationship after the age of six are
August Kubizek, Eva Braun,
and Albert Speer. Hitler and Kubizek were compan-
ions for three years,
beginning when they were both sixteen. KubizekÕs memoir of
Hitler (1955) shows that
his relationship to Hitler was not that of friend but of
adoring admirer. Kubizek
describes Hitler as a compulsive talker, brooking no
interruptions, let alone
any disagreement. Lacking any other listeners at this age,
Hitler used Kubizek as a
sounding board.
Speer,
an architect-engineer, was closest to Hitler among his officials during
the last years of World War
II. In an interview after the war, Speer revealed that
although he spent countless
hours with Hitler, there was no personal relationship
between them (Bromberg and
Small, 1983: 112): ÔIf Hitler had friends, I would
have been his friendÕ.
Her
diary (Bromberg and Small, 1983: 107–8) shows that Eva Braun, HitlerÕs
mistress, came no closer
than Kubizek or Speer. For most of the 15-year relation-
ship, he attempted to keep
it hidden, confining her to her rooms during meetings
with others. Afew entries
suggest the tone of the whole diary. In 1935, when she
was 23 and Hitler 46, she
complained that she felt imprisoned, that she got nothing
from their sexual
relationship, and that she felt desperately insecure: ÔHe is only
using me for definite
purposesÕ(11 March). Most of the women with whom Hitler
had sexual relations either
attempted or committed suicide (Small and Bromberg,
1983: 125, count seven such
relationships, with three of them attempting, and
three completing suicide).
Eva Braun made two such attempts.
In
1942, Hitler inadvertently suggested his isolation from Eva. Hearing of the
death of one of his
officials, Fritz Todt, chief of armaments, he said that he was now
deprived of Ôthe only two
human beings among all those around me to whom I
have been truly and
inwardly attached: Dr. Todt is dead and Hess has flown away
from me!Õ(Toland, 1976:
666.) As Bromberg and Small (1983) note, this statement
leaves Eva out entirely,
mentioning instead Ôa remote man who could rarely be
induced to sit at HitlerÕs
table and a man he could not bear to converse with,
denounced as crazy, and
wished deadÕ(p. l50).
Neither
as a soldier nor as a politician did Hitler have close attachments. His
experience as an enlisted
man in the Army during World War I is illustrative.
Although he was a dedicated
soldier who demonstrated fearlessness in battle, he
was a ÔlonerÕ. He had no
intimates. This may be one of the reasons that although he
was decorated for bravery,
he was little promoted after four years. He left the army
at the rank of lance
corporal, the equivalent of an American private first class. In his
evaluations, he was
described as lacking in leadership.
After
becoming the leader of the Nazi party, he moved no closer to human
relationships. A
description of his campaign the year before gaining power is
representative (Small and
Bromberg, 1983, l08):
[In the campaign, Hitler]
had almost no real contact with people, not even
with his associates, who
felt they were touring with a performer. ... He
remained a lone wolf,
now... more distant from his senior associates, and
contemptuous of them.
Although the adored leader
of millions of people, Hitler apparently had no secure
bond with anyone after the
age of six.
Application
If it proves to be the case
that the silence/violence pattern arises out of anger,
repression of vulnerable
emotions and lack of bonds, and that this pattern is much
more prevalent in men than
in women, what would be the practical implications?
Obviously
one direction would be for men to unlearn their suppression of the
vulnerable emotions,
express anger rather than act it out, and to bond to at least one
other person. Reviewing
events of oneÕs day, as indicated above, can be a par-
ticularly simple and
effective way of moving toward all three of these goals.
However, even if most men
agreed with this direction, which they donÕt, it would
still take a long time to
see effective change. By adulthood, the s/v pattern is com-
pulsive, as is the
repression of the vulnerable emotions, compulsive anger and
isolation from others. It
would take considerable time, energy, and skill to change
this pattern.
In
the meantime, it might be practical to use the difference between men and
women in our political
structures. It is possible that electing/appointing women to
high office, rather than
men, might be a step, on the average, of slowing down the
leap into war and violence.
There are exceptions, of course, like Margaret
Thatcher, who manipulated
collective emotions as skilfully as any man. But most
women are at least somewhat
less easy with this kind of exploitation than our
present leaders,
hypermasculine men. Women also would be less trigger happy
then men, who have a
tendency to fight first and ask questions later.
In
an unpublished paper, Hochschild (n.d.) has proposed that large numbers of
working class men support
the Bush regime, even though its policies are against the
interests of their class.
She argues that the reason for their support is emotional,
rather than economic. They
admire, and wish to emulate BushÕs style of meeting
threat with aggression
rather than with negotiation and compromise. His hypermas-
culine, violent style, is a
reaffirmation of their own. It would appear that this style
is so central to their
identity that it overrides their economic interests.
Each
of the initiatives proposed here may be only one step toward controlling
violence. Having a majority
of leaders be women, rather then men, for instance,
seems a long way away. In Lysistrata, a drama from ancient Greece, women
joined together to deny sex
to men who fought. Perhaps modern women might
take note, not only to
lessen war directly, but also indirectly, to encourage men to
vote for women, or at
least, less arrogant leaders.
Note
This article revises my
earlier approach to male violence (2003) in several ways, particularly
by introducing Ôthe
vulnerable emotionsÕ, and the idea that we need to experience the full
range of emotions. I am
indebted to Suzanne Retzinger for suggesting these two ideas.
References
Belknap, Michael B. 2002 The
Vietnam War on Trial. Lawrence, KS:
University Press of
Kansas.
Bromberg, N., and Small, V.
1983 HitlerÕs Psychopathology. New
York: International
Universities
Press.
Bullock, Alan 1964 Hitler:
A Study in Tyranny. New York: Harper
& Row.
Calley, William Laws 1971 Lieutenant
Calley: His own story (as told to
John Sack).
London:
Hutchinson.
Collins, Randall 1990
ÔViolent conflict and social organisation: some theoretical
implications
of the sociology of warÕ, Amsterdams Sociologisch Tijdschrift(16): 63–87.
Collins, Randall
(forthcoming) Violent Conflict: A Micro-Sociological Theory.
Davidson, E. 1977 The
Making of Adolf Hitler. New York:
Macmillan.
Demos, E.Virginia 1995 Exploring
Affect: The Selected Writings of Silvan S. Tomkins.
Cambridge:
Cambridge University Press.
Doka, K., and T. Martin
1998 ÔMasculine response to lossÕ, Journal of Family Studies, 4:
143–58.
Everett, Arthur 1971 Calley. New York: Dell.
Greenhaw, Wayne 1971 The
Making of a Hero; The Story of Lieut. William Calley Jr.
Louisville:
Touchstone.
Gilbert, G. 1950 The
Psychology of Dictatorship. New York:
Ronald.
Gilligan, James 1996 Violence:
Reflections on a National Epidemic.
New York: Vintage.
Hammer, Richard 1971 The
Court-Martial of Lt. Calley. New
York: Coward, McCann &
Geoghegan
Hersh, Seymour M. 1970 My
Lai 4: a report on the massacre and its aftermath. New York:
Random
House.
Hitler, Adolf 1943 [1927] Mein
Kampf. Boston: Houghton Mifflin.
Hochschild,
Arlie. 2004. Let Them Eat War. European Journal of Psychotherapy, Counseling & Health. 6,
3, December, pp. 1-10.
Krystal, Henry 1988 Integration
and Self-healing : Affect, Trauma, Alexithymia. Hillsdale,
N.J.:
Analytic Press.
Kubizek, A. 1955 The
Young Hitler I Knew. Boston: Houghton
Mifflin.
Lewis, Helen B. 1971 Shame
and Guilt in Neurosis. New York:
International Universities
Press.
Miller, A. 1983 For Your
Own Good. New York: Farrar, Straus,
Giroux.
Parkes, Colin 1998 [1988] Bereavement:
Studies of grief in adult life. 3rd
edn, Madison,
CT:
International Universities Press.
PBS (Undated) The
American Experience: Vietnam,
http://www.pbs.org/wgbh/amex/
vietnam/trenches/mylai.html
Scheff, Thomas 1979 Catharsis
in Ritual, Drama and Healing.
Berkeley, CA: University of
California
Press. (Re-issued in 2000 by iUniverse).
Scheff, Thomas 1994 Bloody
Revenge. Boulder, CO: Westview
(reissued in 1999 by
iUniverse).
Scheff, Thomas 2003 ÔMale
emotions and violence: a case studyÕ, Human Relations, 56:
727–49.
Speer, Albert 1970 Inside
the Third Reich. New York: Macmillan.
Stierlin, H. 1976 Adolf
Hitler: A Family Perspective. New
York: Psychohistory Press.
Taylor, Graeme J., R. Bagby
and J. Parker 1997 Disorders of affect regulation : alexithymia
in
medical and psychiatric illness. New
York : Cambridge University Press.
Taylor, Shelley, et al.
2000 ÔBiobehavioral responses to stress in females: tend-and-
befriend,
not fight-or-flightÕ, Psychological Review, 107: 411–29.
Toland, John 1976 Adolf
Hitler. Garden City, NY: Doubleday.